The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2)

Free download. Book file PDF easily for everyone and every device. You can download and read online The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) file PDF Book only if you are registered here. And also you can download or read online all Book PDF file that related with The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) book. Happy reading The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) Bookeveryone. Download file Free Book PDF The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) at Complete PDF Library. This Book have some digital formats such us :paperbook, ebook, kindle, epub, fb2 and another formats. Here is The CompletePDF Book Library. It's free to register here to get Book file PDF The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) Pocket Guide.

Santa Fe: School of American Research, pp. Before European Hegemony: the world system AD — Oxford: Oxford University Press. Achebe, C. An image of Africa. Research in African Literatures 9 1 : 2— Adams, W. The French Garden, — New York: Braziller. Adorno, T. On the fetish character in music and the regression of listening. Arato and E. New York: Continuum, pp. Agrest, D.

Architecture from Without: theoretical framings for a critical practice. Agyeman, J. The New Countryside? Ethnicity, nation and exclusion in contemporary rural Britain. London: Policy Press. Ahler, S. Thiessen ed. Islamic archaeology in Iraq: recent excavations at Samarra. World Archaeology 14 3 : — London: Routledge. Alexander, C. Notes on the Synthesis of Form. Alexander, E. Alexander ed. Allen, D. Lewis ed. Custom and Ceramics. Essays presented to Kenneth Barton. Wickham: APE, pp. Fashioning Power: clothing politics and African identities.

Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Alpern, S. What Africans got for their slaves: a master list of European trade goods. History in Africa 5— Amin, A. Citizens of the world: seeing the city as a site of international influence. Harvard International Review 14— Amit, V. Constructing the Field: ethnographic fieldwork in the contemporary world. Reconceptualizing community. Amit ed. Realizing Community: concepts, social relationships and sentiments. London: Routledge, pp. The Trouble with Community: anthropological reflections on movement, identity and collectivity.

London: Pluto Press. Amsbury, C. American Anthropologist 66 4 : — Mestizo Logics: anthropology of identity in Africa and elsewhere. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Anderson, B. Hope for nanotechnology: anticipatory knowledge and the governance of affect. Area 39 2 : — Imagined Communities: reflections on the origin and spread of nationalism. London: Verso. Matter s in social and cultural geography. Geoforum 35 6 : — On geography and materiality. Environment and Planning A 41 2 : — Anderson, K.

Anderson and F. Gale eds Inventing Places: studies in cultural geography. Melbourne: Longman Cheshire, pp. Anderson, P. Intimate distance: fantasy islands and English lakes. Ecumene 8 1 : — Andrevsky, A. Lithics: macroscopic approaches to analysis second edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Andrew Dickson White Museum of Art. Earth Art. Andrews, G. Anon Pennsylvania Dutchman 4: 1. Ansell Pearson, K.

Germinal Life. The difference and repetition of Deleuze. Anzieu, D. Paris: Dunod. Appadurai, A. Gastropolitics in Hindu South Asia. American Ethnologist 8: — The Social Life of Things: commodities in cultural perspective. Introduction: commodities and the politics of value. Appadurai ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. Modernity at Large: cultural dimensions of globalization. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. The globalisation of archaeology and heritage: a discussion with Arjun Appadurai.


  • More Printable Memory Games;
  • The Left Hand of Memory (Redlisted Book 2) by Sara Beaman.
  • Akin To No One.
  • United We Fall;

Journal of Social Archaeology 1 1 : 35— Apter, E. Fetishism as Cultural Discourse. Ardener, E. Social anthropology and the decline of modernism. Overing ed. Reason and Morality. In The Hour of Our Death. New York: Alfred A. Armit, I. Neolithic Settlement in Ireland and Western Britain. Oxford: Oxbow Books. Armstrong, R. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Arnheim, R. The Dynamics of Architectural Form.

Giraffes Silently Slip Onto the Endangered Species List

Berkeley: University of California Press. Ascher, R. Analogy in archaeological interpretation. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology — Ethnography for archeology: a case from the Seri Indians. Ethnology 1 3 : — Time's arrow and the archaeology of a contemporary community.

Chang ed. Settlement and Archaeology. Historical Archaeology 8: 7— How to build a time capsule. Journal of Popular Culture 8 2 : — Ashbee, P. Field archaeology: its origins and development. Fowler ed. Archaeology and the Landscape. London: John Baker, pp. Ashmore, W. Archaeologies of Landscape: contemporary perspectives. Oxford: Blackwell Studies in Social Archaeology. Asplen, L. Going with the flow: living the mangle through environmental management practice.

Pickering and K. Guzik eds The Mangle in Practice: science, society and becoming. Asquith, L. London: Taylor and Francis. Ateljevic, I. Lew, C. Hall and A.

Williams eds A Companion to Tourism. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. Atkin, T. Structure and Meaning in Human Settlements. Attfield, J. Wild Things. Oxford: Berg.

Printable Memory Games : How Well Will You Score?

Aubrey, J. Miscellanies upon the Following Subjects. London: printed for Edward Castle. Austin, G. Reciprocal comparison and African history: tackling conceptual Eurocentrism in the study of Africa's economic past. African Studies Review 1— Bachelard, G. The Poetics of Space trans. New York: Orion Press. Bahrani, Z. Women of Babylon: gender and representation in Mesopotamia.

New York: Routledge. The living house: signifying continuity. Samson ed. The Social Archaeology of Houses. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, pp. Bailey, D. Documenta Praehistorica Ljubljana 15— Balkan Prehistory: exclusion, incorporation and identity. Prehistoric Figurines: representation and corporeality in the Neolithic. Beyond the meaning of Neolithic houses: specific objects and serial repetition. Bailey, A. Whittle and V. Cummings eds unsettling the Neolithic. Oxford: Oxbow, pp. Unsettling the Neolithic. Oxford: Oxbow. Living Well Together: sedentism and mobility in the Balkan Neolithic.

Bailyn, B. Strangers in the Realm: cultural margins of the first British Empire. Baker, F. Writing the Past for the Present. Lampeter: St David's University College. Bakhtin, M. Rabelais and his World trans. Bakker, K. Material worlds? Progress in Human Geography 30 1 : 5— Balfour, H. Evolution of Decorative Art. London: Rivington, Percival and Co. Balsamo, A. Technologies of the Gendered Body. Reading cyborg women.

Durham: Duke University Press. Banerjee, M. The Sari. New Delhi: Roll Books. Bapty, I. Archaeology After Structuralism. Barad, K. Agential realism: feminist interventions in understanding scientific practices. Biagioli ed.

Edited by Dan Hicks and Mary C. Beaudry

The Science Studies Reader. New York: Routledge, pp. Meeting the Universe Halfway: quantum physics and the entanglement of matter and meaning. Baram, U. Marketing Heritage: archaeology and the consumption of the past. Barber, B. Consumed: how markets corrupt children, infantilize adults, and swallow citizens whole. New York: W. Barclay, A. The Early Neolithic pottery and fired clay. Benson and A. Oxford: Oxbow Books, pp. Barley, M. The English Farmhouse and Cottage.

London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Barnes, A.

Geek Book Club 006 - 'The Left Hand of Darkness' by Ursula K. Le Guin

The difference between natural and human flaking on prehistoric flint implements. American Anthropologist 99— Barnes, B. Interests and the Growth of Knowledge. Scientific Knowledge: a sociological analysis. Barnes, T. Situating economic geographical teaching. Journal of Geography in Higher Education 30 3 : — Barnett, C. Geoforum 38 6 : — Production and distribution of early pottery in the west Mediterranean. Kingery ed. Barnwell, P. Houses and the Hearth Tax: the later Stuart house and society.

York: Council for British Archaeology. Barraud, C. Of relations and the dead: four societies viewed from the angle of their exchanges. Barrett, J. Contextual archaeology. Antiquity — Fields of discourse: reconstituting a social archaeology. Critique of Anthropology 7 3 : 5— The living, the dead and the ancestors: Neolithic and Early Bronze Age mortuary practices. Barrett and I. Sheffield: Department of Archaeology and Prehistory, pp.

Fragments from Antiquity: an archaeology of social life in Britain, — The politics of scale and the experience of distance: the Bronze Age world system. Larsson and B. Stockholm: Almquist and Wiksell, pp. A thesis on agency. Dobres and J. Robb eds Agency in Archaeology. Intimacy and structural transformation: Giddens and archaeology.

Holtorf and H. Karlsson eds Philosophy and Archaeological Practice. Gothenburg: Bricoleur Press, pp. The effect of the slave trade on the Bassar ironworking society, Togo. DeCorse ed. West Africa during the Atlantic Slave Trade. Archaeological perspectives. London: Leicester University Press, pp.

Barry, A. Political Machines: governing a technological society. London: The Athlone Press. Logics of interdisciplinarity. Economy and Society 37 1 : 20— Barstow, A. Witchcraze: a new history of the European witch hunts. San Francisco: HarperCollins. Barth, F. Ethnic Groups and Boundaries. Boston: Little Brown. Barth ed. Boston: Little Brown, pp. Barthes, R. Elements of Semiology trans. Lavers and C. New York: Hill and Wang. The Fashion System trans. Ward and R. Mythologies trans.

Navigation menu

Lavers; orig. Image-Music-Text trans. London: Fontana. Ethnologia Scandinavica — Bateson, G. Steps to an Ecology of Mind. London: Granada Press. On the Bones of the Serpent: person, memory and mortality in Sabarl society. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Battaglia, D. Problematizing the self: a thematic introduction.

Battaglia ed. Berkeley: University of California Press, pp. Baudrillard, J. Simulations trans. Foss, P. Patton and P. New York: Semiotext e. The System of Objects trans. The Consumer Society: myths and structures trans. London: Sage. Bauer, A. Is what you see all you get? Recognizing meaning in archaeology. Journal of Social Archaeology 2 1 : 37— Baum, R. Bauman, Z. Liquid Modernity. Cambridge: Polity Press. Bausinger, H. Folk Culture in a World of Technology trans.

Elke Dettmer. Bayart, F. Global Subjects. A political critique of globalization. Bayerischer Volksfest The Story of the Bayerischer Volksfest Verein. Bayrd, N. New York: Newsweek. BBC News a. Crane barges sail to Napoli's aid. BBC News b. Looting victim visits Napoli site. Beardsley, J. Earthworks and Beyond: contemporary art in the landscape fourth edition. New York: Abbeville. Beaudry, M. Review essay: exploring gender relations in historical America. American Anthropologist 97 3 : — Reinventing Historical Archaeology. De Cunzo and B. Winterthur, DE: Winterthur Museum, pp.

Beck, B. Animal Tool Behaviour. Becker, G. Accounting for Tastes. Becker, M. Pennsylvania Archaeologist 48 1—2 : 1— Earth offering among the Classic Period lowland Maya: burials and caches as ritual deposits. An upper limit of 20 min was assigned for performance time if an individual failed to find the food reward within the duration. To avoid observer bias, the videos were analyzed in blind i. The community sampling and zebrafish collections were not made inside any reserved forest or protected area. The fish caught during community sampling were identified, recorded, and put back into the water bodies.

No animals were euthanized or sacrificed during any part of the study, and behavioral observations were conducted without any chemical treatment on individuals. At the end of the experiments, all zebrafish were transferred to stock tanks in the laboratory, where they continue to be maintained.

All data analyses were conducted using StatistiXL Version 1. Prior to analysis, behavioral data on performance and number of mistakes was square-root transformed to normalize error distribution. Separate linear mixed models LMMs were used for performance time and number of mistakes made by individuals response variables in R Version 3. Sex, body size, population, trial, and interaction effects were taken as fixed effects and individual IDs as random effect, to account for repeated measures of the same individuals across trials.

Factors that were found to be statistically significant were further analyzed using nonparametric Kruskal—Wallis and Mann—Whitney tests. In order to test for memory of individuals within a population, the performance time and number of mistakes committed during eighth trial and test was compared using Wilcoxon paired-samples test. Post hoc test results for comparison of fifth and eighth trial performances across populations. Comparison of mean number of mistakes made by males and females of a Seripetkalwa SK and b Kaushalya KA populations during training.

Shop now and earn 2 points per $1

Males made significantly fewer mistakes than females. Bars represent standard errors. Tests for memory. Comparison of a mean performance time and b mean number of mistakes made during the eighth trial and test trial. Comparisons were made between performance time and number of mistakes made during eighth and test trial within populations. Our results showed that performance of individuals improved see Fig. Individuals across populations differed in their performance rates, as indicated by the significant interaction between population and trial. Though the populations started with a similar initial performance in the maze, their performances varied as trials progressed.

The mean performance time of population WA was higher than other populations throughout the training see Fig. Therefore, the performance of WA varied the most among the four populations. Our findings emphasize the plausible role of environmental factors such as complexity of the native habitat, predation, and flow conditions in explaining differences in performance in a maze. Learning and memory processes are fine-tuned within a population to suit specific environmental requirements that the animals encounter Brydges et al.

Habitats that are spatially complex would provide for an enriched rearing environment for development of increased neural plasticity and enhanced cognitive behavioral processes Salvanes et al. In our study, the natural habitat of WA population was structurally least complex see Table 1 , and therefore lack of enrichment during early rearing could have resulted in relatively weaker learning abilities. Therefore, comparative studies with more replication of populations from each kind of habitat are warranted to disentangle the role of each of these factors in shaping learning abilities.

However, in order to be able to relate habitat instability to differences in learning abilities among zebrafish populations, the reported habitats would need seasonal sampling. The differences observed in learning could be attributed to confounding effects of other factors as well. Individuals across populations could differ in swimming speed, which could affect the time they took to find the food. Differences in activity and exploratory tendencies between individuals of populations could amount to differences in abilities to solve the spatial task. Fish belonging to high-flow habitats have high metabolic rate and higher energy needs that could result in increased foraging requirement Killen, Glazier, et al.

Since the task involved finding a food reward, individuals with higher metabolic rate could be expected to have greater urge to locate the same during subsequent trials. Again, since wild-collected fish could have been of different ages, this could have potentially affected their learning abilities as well. Further, the interpopulation differences could have been mediated by different degrees of cerebral lateralization among populations through turn biases, and therefore some populations could have performed better than the others.

However, the extent to which differences in brain lateralization among wild zebrafish may translate to turn biases within populations remain to be tested. Although our results showed no significant effect of sex and body size on performance time, there were sex differences in the number of mistakes made. Males made significantly fewer mistakes than females in two populations, KA and SK.

In our study, differences in number of mistakes committed could be linked to difference in precision and speed with which the color-association task was learned. Such variations could be population specific, and therefore we observed males to have made fewer mistakes than females only within two populations. Further studies testing divergences in laterality and color preference between sexes and among wild populations are required to disentangle the interplay of these factors in influencing performance in maze.

In tests for memory, individuals of populations KA and AS showed stronger abilities to remember the learnt task see Figs. On the other hand, memory of the task was significantly reduced for individuals of the SK population, whereas WA, while having shown weakest learning abilities in the maze, remembered the task.

Memory retention is an energetically expensive process and develops in organisms that inhabit environments that demand its requirement Dukas, ; Odling-Smee et al. Spatially complex habitats would promote development of increased neural plasticity and enhanced cognitive abilities Salvanes et al. The ability to learn and remember the association of various cues for locating foraging patches or shelters in composite topographies would be favored by natural selection.

Therefore, KA and AS individuals, which occurred in habitats with comparatively greater substrate and vegetation diversity, exhibited stronger memory abilities compared to individuals from SK and WA, which occurred in spatially simple habitats. The role of habitat stability in influencing memory was beyond the scope of this study, as habitat information was obtained from a single sampling survey.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that differences in learning and memory among wild zebrafish could be caused by a combination of habitat-related and morphological and physiological factors. The strategies adopted by individuals are likely to be adaptively optimized for survival in the immediate surroundings. Whether these responses are phenotypically plastic or genetically determined and therefore fixed within populations needs to be investigated.

Further experiments on heritability of these traits across populations are warranted to tease apart the roles played by either of these factors. The authors express sincere gratitude to Rohitashva Shukla for assistance during collection of wild populations and ecological data from the study locations.

The authors also thank the local fishermen for help during collections of zebrafish. Skip to main content Skip to sections. Advertisement Hide. Download PDF. Divergences in learning and memory among wild zebrafish: Do sex and body size play a role? Article First Online: 19 October The details for habitat measurements of the four collection sites are illustrated in Table 1.

Table 1 Environmental and ecological variables of the four water bodies. A square shaped arena The square tank consisted of an inner square-shaped layer with sides measuring The sides of the inner layer were separated from corresponding sides of the outer wall of the tank by 7. The edges of the inner square were connected to the diagonal ends of the tank walls by cm connections, thereby forming four separate chambers. Each of the four chambers had a main door leading to the center of the test arena. Each chamber had three sections separated by two windows openings as shown.

A removable colored frame red, yellow, green, or blue identified each main door. A food reward associated with an artificial plant was always placed inside the right-hand section of the red-door chamber see Fig. Hence, the choice of the red-door chamber for reward was purely based on random selection. The position of food reward and plant within the red-door chamber was fixed and never changed. A test fish would have to explore the maze and locate the food inside the right-side section of the red-door chamber.

The task in question therefore was composed of a major color-association component followed by a turn component. The outer wall of the testing tank was covered on all sides with brown cardboard paper to prevent any local external cues from affecting fish behavior.


  • The Paddock: The Heart of the Racetrack.
  • Accessibility Navigation.
  • Giraffes Silently Slip Onto the Endangered Species List | Smart News | Smithsonian.

The walls of the inner layer and the diagonal connections were all opaque. The maze was positioned under a white ceiling and uniform lighting conditions. Open image in new window. We selected the first and the eighth trials as these were the initial and final tests for performance for post hoc comparisons. Additionally we also compared the fifth trial performance as it was roughly halfway through the training. Therefore, the performance of population WA was most different from the other three populations during the 5th and the 8th days of training.

In tests for memory based on comparisons of performance and number of mistakes on eighth and the test trial see Fig. Table 2 Post hoc test results for comparison of fifth and eighth trial performances across populations. Table 3 Results of Wilcoxon paired-sample test for memory. Arthur, D. Spatial and non-spatial visual discrimination learning in zebrafish Danio rerio. Animal Cognition , 4 2 , — CrossRef Google Scholar.

Arunachalam, M. Natural history of zebrafish Danio rerio in India. Zebrafish , 10 , 1— Bates, D. R package version 0. Google Scholar. Brown, B. You can now find red data books of the lichens of Britain, the organisms of Malta, or the threatened birds of the United States. The books were also read in surprising ways. For a listing of standardised data and references, the response of readers could be unexpectedly emotional. Here, for example, is the primatologist Russell Mittermeier , recalling his first encounter:. I still have fond memories of receiving in the mail my copy of the first Red Data Book… I was about 20 when I first received this publication, and it had a profound impact on me.

I pored over every page, reading each one dozens of times, feeling awful about those species that were severely endangered, and resolving to dedicate my career to doing something on their behalf quotation from IUCN Red List , p. The reason I am reading the Red Data Books is because I am tracing how science was used to redefine categories of threat to species in the twentieth century. The redefinition of the criteria for inclusion, basing them on quantitative population biology, is a later story, and is the main focus of my historical investigation.

Do get in touch — jonathan. Nevertheless, the Red Data Books of are the key texts of this project to assess and categorise the threatened wildlife of the world. The sheet describes some characteristics, a few scattered distribution facts, and possible reasons for decline assuming it was ever common.

It is a standardised template, deliberately so: gaps were left in plain view to encourage others to fill them with data.

Divergences in learning and memory among wild zebrafish: Do sex and body size play a role?

There almost no green sheets. There are fifteen pigeons and doves pigeonholed in volume 2. The key for decoding these is as follows.

admin